Guest guest Posted January 26, 2004 Report Share Posted January 26, 2004 http://hfn-usa.com/articles/040126researchstudies.htm How Can We Trust Research Studies whenScientists Often Have a Conflict of Interest?Just because a scientific study exists,doesn't make it reliable. This adage becameexcruciatingly clear in the December 7, 2003Los Angeles Times expos‚ of officials at theNational Institute of Health (NIH), who haveworked as consultants for companies whosedrugs were linked to the deaths of patientstaking part in NIH studies. According to thereport, it's also become common forscientists who work for the NIH to consult atprivate companies for stock options and/orhefty consulting fees.1The consulting deals between drug companiesand employees at the NIH, however, have goneall but unnoticed, the Times said, becausethe NIH allows more than 94 percent of itstop-paid employees to keep their consultingincome confidential.Interviews and corporate and federal recordsobtained by the Los Angeles Times documenthundreds of consulting payments to rankingNIH officials. Here are a few:Dr. Stephen I. Katz, director of the NIH'sNational Institute of Arthritis andMusculoskeletal and Skin Diseases, collectedbetween $476,369 and $616,365 in company feesin the last decade. One company paid him morethan $140,000 in consulting fees, and went onto win $1.7 million in grants from hisinstitute.Dr. Ronald N. Germain, deputy director of amajor laboratory at the National Institute ofAllergy and Infectious Diseases, hascollected more than $1.4 million in companyconsulting fees in the last 11 years, plusstock options. One of the companiescollaborated with his laboratory on research.The founder of another of the companiesworked with Germain on a separate NIH-sponsored project.Dr. Richard C. Eastman, the NIH's topdiabetes researcher in 1997, wrote to theFood and Drug Administration that yeardefending a product without disclosing in hisletter that he was a paid consultant to themanufacturer. Eastman's letter said the riskof liver failure from the drug was " veryminimal. " Six months later, a patient, AudreyLaRue Jones, who was taking the drug in anNIH study that Eastman oversaw, sufferedsudden liver failure and died. Liver expertsfound that the drug probably caused the liverfailure.Medical ethicists have said the consultingarrangements represent a clear conflict ofinterest. The problem is, a scientist whogets a lot of money in consulting fees isgoing to want to make sure the company payinghim does very well, and sometimes thatresults in the death of a researchparticipant.Death of a patient had no impact oncontinuance of a studyJamie Ann Jackson, who had been listed as " Subject No. 4 " in a NIH study of thetreatment of kidney inflammation related tolupus, died because of a complicationinvolving the drug Fludara, made by ScheringAG. As it turns out, Dr. Stephen I. Katz, thesenior NIH official involved in the study,was a paid consultant for Schering AG. Katzcould have stopped the study immediatelyafter Jackson's death or warned doctorsoutside the NIH who were prescribing the drugfor similar disorders. But he did neither,because, as the Times reported, either stepmight have threatened the market potentialfor Schering AG's drug.According to the Times, in an articlepublished in the May 2001 issue of thejournal Pharmacotherapy, the doctors, threefrom Katz's institute, wrote that Fludara " was well tolerated " and thanked the companyfor providing the drug and " analyticalsupport. " It wasn't until December, 2003, 4 1/2 yearsafter Jackson died, that these same doctorspublished a full-length article in thejournal Transfusion describing her death. " Such dual roles - federal research leaderand drug company consultant - areincreasingly common at the NIH, an agencyonce known for independent scientific inquiryon behalf of a single client: the public, " writes David Willman, author of the Timesreport. And, in fact, conflicts of interestamong university medical researchers havealso received wide attention in recent years.Private consulting fees distortinterpretation of study resultsDr. Curt D. Furberg, former head of clinicaltrials at the National Heart, Lung and BloodInstitute, is quoted in the Times article assaying " Science should be for the sake ofgaining knowledge and looking for the truth.There should be no other factors involvedthat can introduce bias on decision-making. " Private consulting fees tempt governmentscientists to pursue less-deserving researchand to `put a spin on their interpretation'of study results, " he adds.How do we find credible research?So, how can we trust the interpretations of astudy knowing that scientists sometimesdistort the results to favor their clients?And what about the study's participants? Howcan they be sure the experimental treatmentsthey receive are chosen on merit and notbecause of what a researcher stands to gain?With the amount of time and planning thatgoes into designing a study to stand up toscientific standards, it's absurd to ignorethe human element. The more background on thepeople, the institute, or organization thatperformed or funded the research, the betterwe can assess the reliability of the data.Here are some guidelines we at SmartPublications use when assessing studies: Who funded the study?It's easy to automatically disregard a studythat has been funded by a company that wantsto promote its products, but we realize thata company isn't going to spend a lot of moneyon research that makes them look bad. Theresearch, however, becomes more valid if theresults appear in a peer-reviewed journalbecause that means the methods andconclusions will have been criticallyexamined by other experts in the field. Ofcourse, we're skeptical if a particularscientist's work is always favorable to theorganization that sponsored the study.What kind of study is it?1) Epidemiological studies are observationalstudies, best at identifying powerfulassociations, like the one between smokingand lung cancer. They can't establish causeand effect-such as Alzheimer's disease iscaused by cooking in aluminum pots-but rathercan only suggest a relationship between twofactors or events. When the relationshipbetween factors is weaker, like the linkbetween smoking and breast cancer,epidemiological studies are less reliable andoften produce contradictory results. Also,the larger the study and the longer it hasbeen carried out-such as the 45-year-oldFramingham Heart Study and the Nurses HealthStudy-the more certain you can be of thefindings, becauseresearchers can better account for factorsthat could lead to a bogus conclusion.2) Clinical trials use people as subjects andrandomly assign people to two treatmentgroups. In a blind or single-blind trial thesubjects don't know whether they arereceiving the treatment or a placebo untilthe study is completed. Placebos aretreatments that appear identical to the realtreatment, but contain none of the activeingredients being studied. Using placeboshelps eliminate results that may be due tothe subject's expectation that something issupposed to happen. In a double-blind trialneither the subjects nor the researchers areaware of the treatment being used. This typeof study provides the greatest precisionbecause it removes any possibility ofexperimental bias.3) Laboratory experiments are done in vitroon cells, or tissue samples in culture, or invivo on laboratory animals -- usuallyrodents. They can be tightly controlled, inthat the scientists can make sure thecompared groups are genetically identical andthe conditions to which they are exposed(except for the factor being studied) areidentical. But, as Jane Brody points out inher article, ( " Personal Health: A Study Guideto Scientific Studies " New York Times, Aug.11, 1998) " It is a long way from the testtube or laboratory mouse to man or woman. Thefact that all people do not have the samegenes, hormones, metabolism or lifecircumstances may greatly modify the effects.Or human biology may be such that whateverhappens in a test tube or lower animal wouldnot apply to us. " 2Has the study been peer-reviewed?After receiving funding and completing theresearch, researchers write a manuscript andsubmit it for publication. The journal sendsout the manuscript for peer review, a processin which several scientists with expertise inthe area assess its worthiness andimportance. The manuscript is not acceptedfor publication until the reviewers aresatisfied that the methods and conclusionsare valid and worthwhile.Conclusion:Finally, in looking at scientific studies, werealize that a single study does not mean asubstance or nutrient is either beneficial orhazardous. As Jane Brody writes, " No matterhow reputable the scientist or theinstitution where the research was done orhow compelling the findings appear to be,they must be confirmed by one or moreindependent studies before scientists willaccept them as gospel. " Before the scientific community and publicaccept the benefits and advantages of aparticular substance or supplement, it'snecessary for it to be studied repeatedlyfrom various angles, by more than oneresearch team. Science is often a guessinggame, but with ethical researchers insuringthat scientific measures are put in place, itis possible to get valid results that havebroad and significant implications for thehealth and welfare of mankind.References:1. Willman, David. " Stealth Merger: DrugCompanies and Government Medical Research " Los Angeles Times, December 7, 2003.2. Brody, Jane E. " Personal Health: A StudyGuide to Scientific Studies. " New York Times,August 11, 1998. SiteBuilder - Free web site building tool. Try it! Quote Link to comment Share on other sites More sharing options...
Recommended Posts
Join the conversation
You are posting as a guest. If you have an account, sign in now to post with your account.
Note: Your post will require moderator approval before it will be visible.