Guest guest Posted December 11, 2003 Report Share Posted December 11, 2003 PART 1 Nutrition Vol: 19 Issue: 11-12, November - December, 2003 pp: 994-996 Article Full Text “Brain-specific” nutrients: a memory cure?*1 Mark A. McDaniela, , , , Steven F. Maierb, Gilles O. Einsteinc a. Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, New Mexico , USA b. Department of Psychology, University of Colorado, Boulder, Colorado , USA c. Department of Psychology, Furman University, Greenville, South Carolina , USA Article Outline: 1. Objective 2. Results 3. Conclusions 1. Introduction 2. Nootropics, the aging brain, and neural bases of learning and memory a. The aging brain b. The neural basis of learning and memory 3. Phosphatidylserine a. Mechanisms and animal studies b. Controlled human studies i. Effects on patients with moderate cognitive impairments ii. Effects on normal older adults iii. Safety c. Summary 4. Choline a. Mechanisms and animal studies b. Controlled human studies i. Phosphatidylcholine ii. Citicoline iii. Safety c. Summary 5. Piracetam a. Mechanisms and animal studies b. Controlled human studies i. Effects on patients with probable Alzheimer´s disease ii. Effects in other populations iii. Safety c. Summary 6. Vinpocetine a. Mechanisms and animal studies b. Controlled human studies i. Effects on patients with cognitive impairments ii. Effects on normal younger adults iii. Safety c. Summary 7. Acetyl-l-carnitine a. Mechanisms and animal studies b. Controlled human studies i. Effects on patients with probable Alzheimer´s disease ii. Effects in other populations iii. Safety c. Summary 8. Antioxidants a. Mechanisms and animal studies b. Controlled human studies i. Effects on normal younger adults ii. Effects on patients with brain pathology iii. Safety c. Summary 9. Future work and more fine-grained analyses of memory 10. Ginkgo and ginseng a. Estrogen and related hormones 11. Summary and concluding remarks Acknowledgements References Abstract 1. Objective We review the experimental evaluations of several widely marketed nonprescription compounds claimed to be memory enhancers and treatments for age-related memory decline. We generally limit our review to double-blind placebo-controlled studies. The compounds examined are phosphatidylserine (PS), phosphatidylcholine (PC), citicoline, piracetam, vinpocetine, acetyl-L-carnitine (ALC), and antioxidants (particularly vitamin E). 2. Results In animals, PS has been shown to attenuate many neuronal effects of aging, and to restore normal memory on a variety of tasks. Preliminary findings with humans, though, are limited. For older adults with probable Alzheimer´s disease, a single study failed to demonstrate positive effects of PS on memory performance. For older adults with moderate cognitive impairment, PS has produced consistently modest increases in recall of word lists. Positive effects have not been as consistently reported for other memory tests. There is one report of consistent benefits across a number of memory tests for a subset of normal adults who performed more poorly than their peers at baseline. The choline compounds PC and citicoline are thought to promote synthesis and transmission of neurotransmitters important to memory. PC has not proven effective for improving memory in patients with probable Alzheimer´s disease. The issue remains open for older adults without serious degenerative neural disease. Research on citicoline is practically nonexistent, but one study reported a robust improvement in story recall for a small sample of normally aging older adults who scored lower than their peers in baseline testing. Animal studies suggest that piracetam may improve neuronal efficiency, facilitate activity in neurotransmitter systems, and combat the age-related decrease in receptors on the neuronal membrane. However, for patients with probable Alzheimer´s disease, as well as for adults with age-associated memory impairment, there is no clear-cut support for a mnemonic benefit of piracetam. Vinpocetine increases blood circulation and metabolism in the brain. Animal studies have shown that vinpocetine can reduce the loss of neurons due to decreased blood flow. In three studies of older adults with memory problems associated with poor brain circulation or dementia-related disease, vinpocetine produced significantly more improvement than a placebo in performance on global cognitive tests reflecting attention, concentration, and memory. Effects on episodic memory per se have been tested minimally, if at all. ALC participates in cellular energy production, a process especially important in neurons, and in removal of toxic accumulation of fatty acids. Animal studies show that ALC reverses the age-related decline in the number of neuron membrane receptors. Studies of patients with probable Alzheimer´s disease have reported nominal advantages over a range of memory tests for ALC-treated patients relative to placebo groups. Significant differences have been reported rarely, however. Whether ALC would have mnemonic benefits for aging adults without brain disease is untested as far as we know. Antioxidants help neutralize tissue-damaging free radicals, which become more prevalent as organisms age. It is hypothesized that increasing antioxidant levels in the organism might retard or reverse the damaging effects of free radicals on neurons. Thus far, however, studies have found that vitamin E does not significantly slow down memory decline for Alzheimer´s patients and does not produce significant memory benefits among early Parkinson´s patients. Neither did a combination of vitamins E and C significantly improve college students´ performance on several cognitive tasks. 3. Conclusions In sum, for most of the “brain-specific” nutrients we review, some mildly suggestive effects have been found in preliminary controlled studies using standard psychometric memory assessments or more general tests designed to reveal cognitive impairment. We suggest that future evaluations of the possible memory benefits of these supplements might fruitfully focus on memory processes rather than on memory tests per se. 1. Introduction Memory decline with age has been well documented in the experimental literature for some time.[ 1 ] As Figure 1 shows, in humans this decline may start as early as 30 y of age, with significant decline evidenced by middle age, at least for paired-associate memory. These experimental findings are echoed in people´s personal observations that as they age, their memory seems to get worse. In a sample of 280 people of varying ages whom we queried, we found a threefold increase from the decade of the 30s to the decade of the 40s in the percentage of people reporting that they perceived having some problems with memory. Almost a third of the people in their 40s felt that these problems might be suggestive of Alzheimer´s disease.[ 2 ] Thus, as people age, they appear to have a strong tendency to develop the impression that their memory is declining, an impression that dovetails with the experimental literature. -- (5.41 KB) FIG. 1: Paired-associate learning at various ages. The scores are expressed as a percentage of the maximum score across all ages. Each line shows the results of a separate experiment (identified by the number next to the line). Reprinted from Salthouse[ 117 ] (p. 126) by permission of the author. -- In view of these observations, it is natural that the public has an interest in supplements that are touted to improve memory, forestall memory decline, or help remedy age-related declines in memory. These supplements are easily available and are widespread, dispensed either individually or in combinations as “memory cocktails.” These products are frequently advertised on the radio, in magazines directed at the aging population, and in publications about natural remedies to physical and psychological ailments. It is not surprising, then, that when memory psychologists are engaged in social conversations about memory, they are often asked, “Are there supplements I can take that are supposed to help memory?” and “Do these supplements really work?” These questions are reasonable, and the answers hold importance for individuals who are experiencing age-related memory declines or age-related neural pathology, or who have friends and relatives with such concerns. Unfortunately, these questions cannot be answered by appealing to the mainstream experimental psychology journals, as the issue has not penetrated these journals. Neither can the questions be answered confidently by examining trade books on “brain fitness,” “memory cures,” and so on. In the case of such non-peer-reviewed publications, the cautious reader has reason to question the nature of the database examined, the extent to which the scientific database has been probed, and the leniency with which the data have been interpreted. Further, marketing these products as “memory enhancers” and “brain boosters,” without any proof of efficacy, is legal as long as there are no claims that they are effective in treating or curing disease or illness. Accordingly, the purpose of this review is to identify supplements that have enjoyed reputations as memory enhancers, to consider the possible neurological or physiological mechanisms by which they might affect memory, and to report on the existing behavioral evaluations of their efficacy. At the outset, we were unsure whether such scientific studies existed, and were somewhat skeptical that the claims in the popular press about the memory benefits of these supplements would find any support in well-conducted research. To foreshadow our conclusions, we were somewhat surprised by the number of supplements (in addition to ginkgo) that are hypothesized to increase memory functioning and also by the research findings, which do not justify outright dismissal of some of these supplements. 2. Nootropics, the aging brain, and neural bases of learning and memory The term nootropics (from the Greek “noos” and “tropein,” meaning “mind” and “toward,” respectively) was originally coined to describe the pharmacology of a particular drug, piracetam,[ 3 ] and has now been adopted more generally as a label for the class of agents that 1) improve cognitive functions like memory and learning; 2) provide neuroprotective effects from various insults; 3) do not possess properties of classical excitants, tranquilizers, and antipsychotics; and 4) have very limited or no side effects.[ 4 ] In this article, we review the existing experimental evaluations of several widely marketed nonprescription agents claimed to have nootropic effects. These drugs (mostly nonprescription) and nutrients are featured in the popular press as memory- or cognitive-enhancing supplements, and are recommended as part of treatment regimens at some aging clinics. They include Ginkgo biloba. phosphatidylserine (PS), vinpocetine, acetyl-L-carnitine (ALC), piracetam, choline-related nutrients thought to be involved in producing acetylcholine (ACh), and antioxidant agents like vitamin E. These are often combined into memory-cocktail supplements and sold commercially. For example, the first four nutrients listed have recently been combined into a single cocktail supplement and sold as Memory 2000 (produced by Natural Balance). 2.a. The aging brain The presumed neural benefits of these nootropic agents may articulate well with the neural declines associated with normal aging and with degenerative neural pathologies commonly seen in older adults. The growing evidence suggests at least three prominent global changes in the brain that occur with age. First, the neurons show multiple changes, and neuronal changes are a more decisive hallmark of age than widespread death of neurons.[ 5 ] Briefly, the aging-related neuronal changes include accumulation of nonessential substances (e.g., yellowish brown lipid lipofuscin—“wear and tear” pigment), loss of essential myelin (fatty material around axons; the axon conducts an electrical signal away from the neuron body, and myelin promotes speedy and reliable propagation of the signal), and general shrinkage. With regard to age-related changes in memory and cognitive functioning, it is perhaps significant that lipid lipofuscin accumulates prominently in cortical neurons,[ 5 ] and myelin loss is most notable in the association and limbic cortices (specific areas of the cerebral cortex).[ 6 ] Second, the connections between neurons, not just the neurons themselves, change with age. There is a reduction in the branching of dendrites (fibers on which axons of other neurons terminate) and a decline in the number of properly functioning connections between neurons.[ 5 ] Aging may depress the availability of neurotransmitters such as ACh, and ACh seems to be heavily involved in neuron networks associated with memory. Third, with age the cerebrovascular system shows numerous structural changes, diminishing cerebral blood flow, and declining cerebral blood volume. With extreme shortage or suppression of blood flow, a condition called ischemia exists. As we discuss in the individual sections dedicated to the various nootropic agents, and as we summarize in - Table: [ 1], some nootropics may help stem age-related changes in neurons by providing the essential substances for cell membrane health (e.g., PS, citicoline) or by protecting neurons against toxic effects produced by oxidative processes (e.g., antioxidants) and other sources (e.g., ALC, piracetam). Some nootropics may augment neuronal connections by promoting branching of dendritic spines (PS), maintaining neuron receptors (PS, ALC, piracetam), or stimulating the production or release of ACh (cholines, ALC, piracetam). Other agents may function by increasing blood flow (vinpocetine). -- Table I: THEORETICAL MECHANISMS OF NUTRIENTS CLAIMED TO BE MEMORY ENHANCERS (15.2 KB) -- 2.b. The neural basis of learning and memory Before proceeding, it is necessary to preview how neuronal functions and connections underlie learning and memory. Because learning and memory involve the retention of information over long periods of time, they must be mediated by relatively permanent changes in the networks of neurons that represent the information. Unraveling the mystery of how this occurs has been a fascinating success story of modern science, and the broad outline is as follows. It all begins with the release of a neurotransmitter, the chemical messenger between neurons, from terminals in the axon of a neuron. The neurotransmitter molecules then bind to receptors on the membrane of the dendrites of nearby neurons, thereby initiating a complex cascade of events within those neurons that lead to the permanent changes that are memory. The binding of a neurotransmitter to one type of receptor (ionotropic receptors) allows ions of various kinds to rapidly cross the cell membrane into the neuron. This passage of ions changes the electrical potential between the inside and outside of the neuron and causes the neuron to “fire” an electrical signal. However, this occurs within milliseconds and does not produce a long-term change in the neuron, and thus cannot be the basis of memory. But there is a second type of receptor. The binding of a neurotransmitter to this type of receptor (metabotropic receptors) induces the production of what are called second-messenger molecules (the neurotransmitter is the first messenger) within the neuron. These second messengers travel within the neuron, initiating a large number of different biological reactions and controlling the functioning of the neuron. The reaction of most importance for memory is the activation of a number of different enzymes called kinases. The functioning of any cell is determined by the proteins that are produced in the cell and their activity, and kinases selectively alter the activity of proteins. Kinases can remain active for hours once activated, and so have time to produce many prolonged alterations within the neuron. In addition, some kinases can enter the nucleus and initiate the activation of specific genes, thereby leading to the production of novel proteins and thus an altered neuron—a memory. Some of these new proteins then produce physical growth of the neural fibers that directly interact with other neurons. For example, new spines may form on the dendrites of the neuron, thus strengthening its connection to the neuron that began it all by releasing the neurotransmitter. These new physical structures can be relatively permanent and form the physical basis for a stable memory. Figure 2 provides an illustrative schematic of the neural processes just discussed. -- (7.89 KB) FIG. 2: Illustration of how two neurons communicate. In the neuron that sends the “message” (i.e., the presynaptic neuron), neurotransmitters (the chemical messengers that communicate between neurons) are synthesized and packaged into vesicles. These vesicles are located at terminals at the ends of the neuron´s axon. If the neuron becomes sufficiently depolarized, the transmitter molecules are extruded across the cell membrane and enter the space between this neuron and neurons nearby (the synaptic cleft). The transmitter molecules then bind to receptors on the surface of these postsynaptic neurons (dendrites). There are two main types of receptors: ion-channel and G-protein–coupled receptors ®. The binding of a transmitter to an ion-channel receptor leads the channel to open, allowing specific ions to enter the neuron across the membrane. This is the way in which rapid changes in the postsynaptic neuron are produced. The binding of a transmitter to the surface of a G-protein–coupled receptor leads to alterations in the state of proteins (G) that are coupled to the receptor. This alteration then leads to the production of second-messenger molecules, which can exert both immediate and more prolonged effects on the neuron. For example, as illustrated, these messengers can lead to the activation of substances called protein kinases. These protein kinases can, in turn, enter the nucleus of the neuron and act on transcription factors that regulate the transcription of DNA into RNA. Thus, activation of these receptors can alter the genes that are expressed by the postsynaptic neuron, thereby producing the long-term changes that are involved in memory. -- The compromised communication between neurons that is associated with aging and brain disease may be due to a decrease in the production of neurotransmitters or a deficit in any of the processes involved in the complex cascade of biological events that intervene between the binding of a neurotransmitter to a receptor and long-term alterations in the functional state of the neuron. More specifically, there are likely declines in aspects of the processes within the neuron, such as the activity of kinases, that lead to the long-term, stable changes that form the basis of memory. The theory is that memory decline might be avoided by using nootropic-like agents to slow down neuron and brain-tissue loss and loss of function so as to restore depleted memory-related neural processes. Because the mnemonic effects of these agents seem most likely to emerge in older populations that are at risk for neural impairment, and because the need for nootropic agents is pressing for aging individuals, especially those with dementias, the scientific evaluation of such agents has been almost exclusively conducted with older adults having demonstrated memory impairment. Ideally, a complete understanding and evaluation of the effects of supplements on memory would specify the particular neural or metabolic influence of each supplement; identify age-related changes in neural functioning; delineate the possible effects of age and supplements on particular neuropsychological systems; and link these effects to particular kinds of memory functioning. Unfortunately, none of these issues is well understood, and the experimental human studies have not been guided by this kind of rich theoretical orientation. In our review of the experimental findings, we have attempted to synthesize as much information pertaining to these fundamental issues as the literature allows, and we hope that in so doing we have provided a solid foundation for further systematic research on nootropic supplements. We generally limit our review to double-blind, placebo-controlled studies, as placebo and expectation effects can seriously compromise the interpretation of studies without these experimental safeguards.[ 7 ] Also, because a recent report by Gold et al.[ 8 ] focuses on Ginkgo biloba, we limit discussion of ginkgo to one recent experimental finding. Our primary goal is to examine the various other supplements claimed to have memory benefits. - Table: [ 2]summarizes the results of the human studies we report in the sections that follow. -- Table II: SUMMARY OF HUMAN EXPERIMENTAL FINDINGS (143 KB) -- 3. Phosphatidylserine In recent years, PS has created excitement as a potential “brain-specific” nutrient to help older adults improve declining memory.[ 9 ] It is a naturally occurring phospholipid that is taken into the body as part of the normal diet. Phospholipids are a major component of biological membranes. PS is a minor percentage of the phospholipids that compose biological membranes, but may be especially important in determining neuronal membrane surface potential (the electrical potential at the membrane) and local ionic environment (the mix of electrically charged particles within the neuron.[ 10 ] Thus, PS is informally characterized as a brain-specific nutrient because of its possible importance in neuronal functioning. Like ginkgo, PS can be purchased as an over-the-counter supplement in many groceries and drugstores. PS has stimulated significant interest in Italy as a treatment for age-associated and dementia-related memory impairment and is featured in a tradebook as a memory cure for age-associated memory impairment.[ 9 ] How might PS promote memory functioning? 3.a. Mechanisms and animal studies PS is thought to be especially vital to the neuron membrane. This membrane is particularly important for the communication between neurons. Recall that networks of communicating neurons store memories. Some areas of the neuron membrane contain receptors responsible for receiving the neurotransmitter message from other neurons. Other parts of the neuron membrane allow the neuron to pass the message from one end of the neuron to the other. This process is a fascinating one in which the cell membrane essentially transmits an electrical current from one end of itself to the other. The problem is that as people age, the neuronal membrane changes somewhat in its composition and starts to lose receptors. Also, the receptors that are left begin to lose the capacity to receive messages. It is also possible that the membrane begins to become more “rigid,” so that it cannot easily transmit the electrical charge along the neuron. It is easy to see that if these problems become too severe, neurons simply will not pass on the messages they receive. When communication among neurons is compromised, the neuron networks that store memories will fail, and memory will decline. PS seems to help the neuronal membrane resist these age-related changes in its composition, and possibly even to revitalize itself so that it can reverse some of them. PS within the neuronal membrane is especially important for the activation of a particular kinase—protein kinase C (PKC)—that plays a critical role in learning and memory. As already mentioned, the binding of a neurotransmitter to certain receptors initiates the production of second messengers within the neuron. One of these second messengers acts on PKC within the cytoplasm of the neuron to induce it to move to the cell membrane, where it becomes activated by binding with calcium and PS. That is, PS within the membrane is necessary to activate PKC. PKC has many functions within the neuron, including the activation of genes that are critical in producing the long-term changes involved in memory. PKC also is involved in regulating the release of neurotransmitters from neurons, another critical aspect of the neural process that underlies cognitive function. Neurotransmitter molecules are held in organelles called synaptic vesicles, with several thousand molecules being in a single vesicle. These vesicles are loaded into specialized release sites in the axon terminals called active zones. To release transmitter from the neuron, the vesicle must move up to and fuse with the neuron´s cell membrane, a process called exocytosis. This process is quite complex and involves a large number of proteins. PKC regulates the functioning of a number of these proteins, and so regulates the release of many different types of transmitters, one of which is ACh. It is noteworthy that PKC activity declines with age,[ 11 ] perhaps because of age-related deficits in PS. Research with aging animals has shown that long-term treatment with dietary PS attenuates and perhaps even eliminates many of the neuronal effects of aging. For example, we noted earlier that the growth of dendritic spines is a key substrate of stable long-term memory. There is a loss of dendritic spines with aging, and this loss is prevented by dietary PS.[ 12 ] Treatment with PS has also been reported to counteract the reduction in release of neurotransmitters (e.g., ACh, dopamine, and norepinephrine) that occurs with aging.[ 13 ] Aging not only reduces the amount of neurotransmitter released by neurons, but can also lead to reductions in the numbers of receptors that are present on the membrane surface to receive the neurotransmitter message. This is likely due to reductions in the expression of the genes that code for receptors, a reduction that could easily be caused by reductions in kinase (e.g., PKC) activity. Interestingly, PS has been shown to restore receptor numbers to normal in aged mice.[ 14 ] Also, PS seems to help the neuron membrane maintain its charged state[ 10 ] so that it can transmit its electrical message. Finally, PS may be important for maintaining the general structure and health of the neuron.[ 10, 15 ] Simply put, PS supplements might have beneficial effects on memory by allowing neurons in the neuron networks to keep effectively communicating with one another so that existing memories can be retained and new memories formed. The theory is that as people age, they need to supplement the brain with more PS than they get through their normal diets. Long-term treatment with PS has been reported to restore normal memory in aged animals on a variety of tasks. Aged animals show declines in learning and memory on a wide spectrum of tasks, and PS treatment has been broadly effective. For example, a task called the Morris water maze is used in many studies of aging. In this task, a rat or a mouse is placed in a circular tank of water that has been made opaque. A platform is placed in the tank, but its surface is a few centimeters below the surface of the water so that it is not visible. Rats and mice do not like being in water, and so the animal swims about the tank in an effort to find an escape route. It will, by accident, encounter the platform and climb onto it, thereby escaping the water. The animal is allowed to stay on the platform for a period of time, and then placed in the water again. The platform is always in the same location, and on succeeding trials the rat or mouse is started in different locations within the tank. The outcome is that the animal learns the spatial location of the platform by using cues within the room in which the tank is located, and swims directly to the platform no matter where in the tank the animal is placed. A large amount of research has shown that the rat or mouse forms a spatial map of the maze that it uses to guide its escape, and this map is retained in memory. The animal can be tested days after training, and it will swim right to the hidden platform. The Morris water maze is of special interest because it is very sensitive to the functioning of a particular part of the brain called the hippocampus, a region that is especially vulnerable to age-related declines. Thus, an animal with damage to the hippocampus cannot learn and remember this task. Aging is associated with severe deficits in learning and remembering this task, and these are reversed by PS treatment.[ 16 ] 3.b. Controlled human studies 3.b.i. Effects on patients with moderate cognitive impairments A handful of double-blind, placebo-controlled, multicenter experiments examining the effects of PS on memory performance in older humans have been conducted in Italy.[ 17, 18, 19 ] The subjects in these studies were older adult patients ranging in age generally from 55 to 80 y and displaying moderate cognitive decline as assessed by standard screening tests. Patients with concomitant severe medical conditions, such as depression, chronic alcoholism, and severe Alzheimer´s disease, were excluded, as were patients who were taking medications that might mask or interfere with the possible effects of PS (e.g., other nootropic drugs, barbiturates, antidepressants, antipsychotics). At each center, patients were randomly assigned either to treatment with 300 mg of PS per day (divided into three daily doses of 100 mg each) or to placebo treatment (e.g., corn oil) for periods ranging from 8 to 24 wk. Sample sizes were reasonable, ranging from 87 patients[ 18 ] to 388.[ 17 ] Memory tests were administered prior to treatment, at the conclusion of treatment, and usually at the midpoint of treatment. The various experiments used similar though not identical tasks measuring immediate and delayed recall. Short lists of words (5–15) were first auditorily presented at brisk rates (usually one word every 2 s). Usually the list (or nonrecalled items of the list) was re-presented to allow multiple recall trials, and a total recall score, representing combined performance across all trials, was calculated. Typically the pretreatment recall levels were used as a covariate, providing a sensitive evaluation of treatment effects. In all these experiments, PS consistently and significantly improved total recall relative to the placebo treatment for this subject population. However, the effects were also uniformly modest. More precisely, across the studies the proportion of words recalled for the placebo groups ranged from 0.36 to 0.60. The PS treatment increased the proportion of recall by just under 0.03 to just over 0.06 across the studies. This proportion translates into an increase in total recall of between one and two words. In one case, this increase was the result of a dynamic whereby the placebo group´s recall decreased by less than a word from pretreatment to the end of treatment, and the PS group´s recall increased by less than a word.[ 19 ] Villardita et al.[ 19 ] also reported significant benefits of PS for digit span (recall of digit lists in either forward or backward order; Palmieri et al.[ 18 ] did not find significant benefits for digit span) and for immediate and delayed “cued semantic verbal memory” tests in which semantically related cues were apparently provided to prompt retrieval of words. Other memory tests in this study did not uniformly show a significant advantage of PS. Briefly, the PS and placebo groups showed no significant difference in immediate and delayed recall of geometric figures or in delayed recall of a 15-item list. This pattern of no or minimal effects of PS on memory tasks other than immediate recall of lists of items was echoed in two additional studies using small numbers of patients. In one study, conducted in the United States, the patients met criteria for probable Alzheimer´s disease (51 patients),[ 20 ] and in the other study, conducted in Germany, they had a diagnosis of primary degenerative dementia (33 patients).[ 21 ] The treatment periods and dosage levels were the same as in the Italian studies. Unlike the Italian researchers, Engel et al. used a design in which each participant was tested once after PS treatment and once after placebo treatment (double crossover design), allowing within-subjects comparison of PS with placebo treatment. In this study, none of the three memory tests, including prose and associative-memory tests, showed benefits of an 8-wk 300-mg/ day PS treatment regimen. Similarly, in the study by Crook et al.,[ 20 ] none of the 10 objective cognitive and memory tests showed effects of a 12-wk 300-mg/d PS treatment. Several of the memory items on an interview-based scale (a clinical global improvement scale) showed a benefit of PS treatment. For a subsample of 33 patients with mildest impairment (scores of 19–23 on the Mini-Mental State Examination. MMSE; lower scores on this measure indicate more severe deficits), only a single objective test (one that involved associating first and last names) showed a significant benefit of PS at the end of the 3-mo treatment period (though again, several memory-related scale items showed benefits of PS). Clearly, as the authors acknowledged, the interpretation of this effect is clouded by concerns about the large number of comparisons conducted. Given that they used a P value of 0.05, rather than a more stringent value, for establishing significance, the probability of a type 1 error (concluding that a difference exists when it does not) was relatively high. In summary, among older adults with cognitive impairment that can be considered moderate, PS has produced consistently modest increases in memory performance for a particular recall paradigm (quick presentation of relatively short lists of items). There is little evidence of positive memory effects on other memory tests. From all these studies, only one positive mnemonic effect of PS that could be characterized as sizable emerged. For the cued semantic verbal memory test, the PS group recalled about 50% more items than the placebo group after 3 mo of treatment (proportion of items recalled was 0.64 versus 0.44).[ 19 ] 3.b.ii. Effects on normal older adults In a double-blind, placebo-controlled, multicenter study, Crook et al.[ 22 ] investigated the mnemonic effects of PS in a sample of 149 normally aging adults ranging in age from 50 to 75 y. The participants were considered to have age-associated memory impairment (i.e., memory decline associated with normal aging). People with dementia, Alzheimer´s disease, or other neurological disorders associated with cognitive deterioration were excluded from the study. Another feature of this study is that memory testing was conducted 4 wk after the end of the 12-wk treatment, as well as during the treatment (at 3 wk, 6 wk, 9 wk, and 12 wk). Five memory tests related to everyday memory use constituted the primary memory evaluation: learning of name-face associations, delayed recall of the name-face associations, face recognition, telephone-number recall, and recall of misplaced objects. The authors designated these tests as primary on the basis of normative data showing that these tests produce a clear pattern of age-related decline in performance. Several other memory tests that did not show such clear age-related decline were used as well and were designated as secondary (e.g., story recall). Overall, the PS treatment produced modest effects. Acquisition and delayed recall of name-face associations were significantly improved during the first 6 weeks of treatment, but these differences did not persist during the latter half of the 12-wk treatment. Further, these differences were slight in that they represented about a 1-point improvement over a score of just over 9 (1 point was given for every name correctly recalled upon being cued with the face). By the end of the treatment, the PS group significantly outperformed the placebo group on only one test, the face-recognition test. More consistent and long-lasting effects of PS were observed in a subgroup of 57 participants who performed poorly on pretreatment memory tests but similarly to the other participants on the vocabulary subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. For these participants, either immediately at the conclusion of the treatment or at testing 4 wk after treatment, there were significant benefits of PS relative to the control for all the primary memory measures, as well as for story recall. Also, ratings by a psychologist or nurse showed that this cluster of PS-treated participants improved more than the placebo group on several items in a measure of specific cognitive symptoms and overall cognitive status. 3.b.iii. Safety The studies reviewed reported no adverse effects from the PS treatment. In one study, many of the participants were patients on medication, and PS did not interact with any of the pharmaceutical drugs that these patients were taking.[ 17 ] However, patients taking antipsychotics, antidepressants, barbiturates, methyl-dopa, reserpine, and bromocriptine were excluded from the study. Thus, there is no evaluation of possible interactions of PS with all potential pharmaceuticals taken by adults. Crook and Adderly[ 9 ] recommended against taking PS during pregnancy or lactation and cautioned that individuals taking anticoagulant medication should be careful with PS. One major safety-related issue concerns the source of the PS. Most studies used bovine PS, but concerns have since been raised about the possibility of viral contamination of that source. Accordingly, PS derived from soy lecithin is now being sold. One possible controversy is whether plant-derived PS has the same effects as animal-derived PS, although Crook and Adderly[ 9, 23 ] suggested that soy-based and bovine PS produce similar mnemonic effects. 3.c. Summary On the basis of the studies just reviewed, clinical studies without double-blind controls, and clinical observation, some psychologists and medical professionals advocate the use of PS, sometimes along with other supplements like ginkgo, for preventing or reversing memory loss associated with age and age-related dementias.[ 9, 23, 24, 25, 26 ] Some researchers are quite optimistic about the effects of PS. For example. Crook and Adderly[ 9 ] concluded that “PS is effective in delaying and usually reversing age-associated memory impairment” (p. 86). In a review of nutrients for restoring cognitive function. Kidd[ 23 ] claimed that “PS is a phospholipid validated through double-blind trials for improving memory, learning, concentration, word recall, and mood in middle-aged and elderly subjects with dementia or age-related cognitive decline” (p. 144). In light of the studies just reviewed, we believe that these are overly generous interpretations of the scientific evidence. PS does produce effects in the mammalian brain that enhance brain functioning, and it attenuates age-related deficits in learning and memory in a variety of animal paradigms. However, the documented mnemonic effects for PS in humans are limited in a number of critical ways. First, the corpus of studies is small. Second, within this small set of studies, the effects of PS are not consistent across different population groups nor across different types of memory tests. Third, a number of the reported memory increases after PS treatment, though statistically significant, are modest. We are not convinced that the modest increases found would necessarily translate into noticeable differences in memory functioning. Finally, relatively robust effects of PS, in terms of both the degree and the consistency of the improvement across a number of memory tests, seem limited to just one small sample of older adults who had no diagnosed dementias, showed relatively more age-associated memory decline than their peers, were relatively well educated, and scored higher than average on subtests of IQ batteries.[ 22 ] These cautionary remarks notwithstanding, in our opinion these preliminary findings are strong enough to warrant further study and suggest possible foci for investigation. Older adults with relatively severe age-associated memory decline might be fruitfully singled out for further study of possible benefits of PS. More judicious selection of memory tests might be warranted as well. The list-recall paradigm appears to be consistently sensitive to PS effects. Reliable replications of these results would provide a foothold from which to explore and analyze benefits of PS. Failure to find consistent effects on memory in some studies may be due to insensitivity in the memory tests used[ 22 ] or to using tests that do not articulate with the specific memory processes that PS may influence.[ 26 ] Clearly, most, if not all, of the questions concerning possible memory benefits of PS remain unanswered. We cannot rule out the possibility that PS enhances memory for at least some older adults with memory impairment, but we also cannot confidently conclude that PS has specific positive effects on memory. Quote Link to comment Share on other sites More sharing options...
Recommended Posts
Join the conversation
You are posting as a guest. If you have an account, sign in now to post with your account.
Note: Your post will require moderator approval before it will be visible.