Guest guest Posted March 17, 2004 Report Share Posted March 17, 2004 by Dr. Jooyoung Julia Shin, M.D. Introduction Eastern religious and secular groups, such as the Buddhists, Taoists, an= d the Indian Yogis have practiced meditation throughout history in order to achieve cert= ain mental and physical ends; these include muscular relaxation and "clearing" the min= d, as well as the more esoteric union with nature or God. For these practitioners, me= ditation further serves to reduce negative tensions in both conscious and subconsci= ous realms, and facilitates the integration of an individual into her or his p= hysical, social and psychological environment. A variety of these ideas were incorporated i= nto the philosophy of the martial arts as they developed in Asia. In contrast to most exercise cultures, the martial arts actively strive = to develop both the inner and outer individual, guided by a holistic view of human nature. = The union of mind and body lies fundamental to martial art philosophy and practice, w= hich consists of both mental and physical exercises. The practice of meditation = characterizes the martial arts as a psychophysical engagement, as opposed t= o a purely physical activity. Although diverse types of meditation exist, all m= editative techniques attempt to focus attention in a nonanalytical way without discur= sive or discriminating thought. By muting the analytical, reasoning functions of th= e mind one achieves a sort of non-discriminatory or relaxed awareness. In the martial = arts, this state has been given various descriptions such as "Satori,enlightenment,= " or "Zanshin." Eastern philosophers have known for centuries that the practice of medit= ation allows the human mind to transcend thinking processes into a state of thoug= htless awareness. Given the complicated structure of the brain, with its multitude= of neurons, infinite possibilities of synaptic connections, and numerous chemi= cal mediators, this transcendent state may one day have a physiological explana= tion. Indeed, increasing scientific and popular interest in the Eastern practices= of meditation has accumulated significant empirical evidence about the physiol= ogical modifications produced by the practice of meditation; these include metabol= ic, autonomic, encephalographic, and psychological effects. These scientific st= udies clearly show that the meditative state of awareness is distinct from a norm= al everyday awareness bound by logic and reason, and validate the traditional Eastern b= elief that mental function has a direct implication on physical function. The Physiology of Meditation A review of the scientific literature on meditation reveals that its pra= ctice can provide numerous benefits for the martial artist. For example, meditation c= an reduce stress and anxiety, enhance motor reflexes, increase motor control, increas= e exercise tolerance, sharpen perceptions, increase awareness, improve concentration, = maintain health, provide a general positive outlook on life, and foster the developm= ent of a sense of personal meaning in the world. In general, meditation produces a reduction in multiple biological syste= ms, resulting in a state of relaxation. These changes are, in most studies, sig= nificantly different between meditating and non-meditating groups. Benson (1975) argue= s that this physiological response pattern is not unique to meditation per se, but= is common to any passive relaxation procedure. Although some studies have found no physiological or overt behavioral differences between meditation and other = relaxation techniques, it is significant to note that subjects report meditational exp= eriences as more profound and enjoyable than their comparative control groups (Cauthen = & Prymak 1977, Kohr 1977). These subjective differences may have critical rel= evance from a clinical or research perspective. Scientific studies reveal that meditation produces a specific physiologi= cal response pattern that involves various biological systems. The mechanisms most frequ= ently suggested to mediate or produce meditative effects include metabolic, auton= omic, endocrine, neurological, and psychological observations. Precisely how thes= e mechanisms are involved in producing the final pattern of responses is yet = unclear. The vast complexity of biological organization indicates that the physiolog= ical response to meditation probably occurs on a multidimensional, interactive b= asis. Meditation and Metabolism Mental states can markedly alter physiologic function. For example, stre= ssful situations result in a hypermetabolic state, with increased oxygen consumpt= ion, heart rate, and blood pressure. In contrast, the majority of scientific studies s= how meditation to be a wakeful state accompanied by a decreased metabolism. Thi= s generalized decrease in body metabolism manifests with a decreased breathin= g pattern, decreased heart rate, and decreased blood pressure. There is also = a marked decrease in the level of oxygen utilization and carbon dioxide elimination = by muscle. These findings have been verified by an impressive number of studies.[FN1] = Oxygen consumption is generally regarded as a reliable index of physical= activity and arousal. For example, exercise requires an increased consumption of oxy= gen by muscle. During this metabolic process, oxygen is converted to carbon dioxid= e, which is eliminated by the lungs. If the body is starved of oxygen, reduced oxyge= n consumption does not lead to a parallel reduction in carbon dioxide elimina= tion because the cells continue to metabolize the remaining oxygen in the blood.= Therefore, oxygen starvation causes a decrease in the concentration of oxyg= en and an increase in the concentration of carbon dioxide in arterial blood. The r= elative amount of oxygen and carbon dioxide in the blood is called the respiratory = quotient. During normal respiratory processes, this quotient remains constant; in abn= ormal respiratory situations, however, the reduction in available oxygen and incr= ease in carbon dioxide changes the quotient. Wallace et al (1971) found that during= the practice of meditation the amount of carbon dioxide elimination drops in pr= oportion to the amount of oxygen consumed; therefore, the respiratory quotient remai= ns constant. In conclusion, the metabolic changes of meditation arise from a n= atural reduction in metabolic activity at the cellular level, not from a forced re= duction of breathing. Circulation, especially in muscle and brain, is closely related to the m= etabolic requirements of tissues, and is very sensitive and consistent in its respon= se to behavior. A study by Jevning et al (1996) illustrates an interesting redist= ribution in the blood flow of meditators. Blood flow to the kidneys and liver declined in p= ractitioners, with a surprising increase in cardiac output. These changes of blood flow i= mply a marked redistribution of blood flow during meditation. It is hypothesized t= hat most of the distributed circulation must be to the brain, a hypothesis that has bee= n supported by direct estimation of increased relative cerebral blood flow (Herzog et a= l 1990, Jevning et al 1992, Jevning et al 1996). The redistribution of blood flow w= ith an increase in cardiac output has interesting significance for the pattern of = metabolic changes elicited by meditation; although the response to meditation is hypo= metabolic overall, it appears likely that there is a concomitant increase in the meta= bolism of certain tissues. Meditation and the Autonomic Nervous System Skin resistance to electrical current provides a measure of autonomic ne= rvous system reactivity. An increase in the skin resistance of meditators has bee= n reported by several groups.[FN2] Increase in skin resistance indicates a decrease in= skin conduction and a reduction in its fluctuations. It is well established that= skin resistance decreases in states of anxiety or stress, and increases during r= elaxation. The large increases in skin resistance of meditators found in these studies= are impressive. Galvanic skin response, or GSR, was used to measure recovery from stress= ; a study by Orme-Johnson (1973) showed that meditators recovered from stress more qu= ickly than non-meditators. Specifically, habituation of the GSR to stress was fas= ter for meditators than for controls, and meditators made fewer multiple responses = during habituation, indicating greater stability in response to stress. In other e= xperiments, meditators produced fewer spontaneous GSR than their non-meditating control= s, both during and while out of meditation. Spontaneous GSR is defined as spon= taneous fluctuations in skin resistance and the frequency of spontaneous GSR define= s the lability of an individual to stress. For example, the frequency rises with = anger, fear, and increased epinephrine and norepinephrine blood levels. Those individual= s with lower frequencies of spontaneous GSR exhibit more effective behavior in a n= umber of stressful situations, are less impulsive on motor tasks, and have quicker p= erceptions. Rapid GSR habituation and low levels of spontaneous GSR are reported in the= literature to be correlated with physiological and behavioral characteristi= cs associated with good mental health. Therefore, meditation benefits practitioners by de= creasing the frequency of spontaneous GSR. In general, these studies indicate that m= editators possess a more adaptive pattern of stress response than controls. On another level, meditation produces specific neural activation pattern= s involving decreased limbic arousal in the brain (Schwartz 1975). Since the limbic sys= tem contains the hypothalamus, which controls the autonomic nervous system, red= uction in limbic arousal may explain how meditation reduces stress and increases a= utonomic stability to stress. Ultimately, meditation strengthens and enhances the ab= ility to cope with stress. Meditation and the Endocrine System Based upon the metabolic characteristics of meditation and the subjectiv= e reports of meditators, several studies were initiated in order to ascertain whether= the blood levels of stress-related chemicals decreased during this practice. A number= of endocrine reactions have been identified in the meditative response pattern= , including reduced blood levels of lactate, cortisol, and epinephrine (Wallace 1970, S= udsuang et al 1991). The reductions in these blood chemicals denote a state of decreas= ed tension and anxiety. For example, the infusion of lactate can produce anxiety sympt= oms in normal subjects (Wallace et al 1971); the decrease in lactate concentration= during and after meditation may explain the subjective feelings of wakeful relaxation.= These studies further reveal that the reduction in stress-related chemicals persi= sts into the post-meditation period. The most likely explanation of these results seems = to be that the long-term practice of meditation develops a psychophysiological respons= e of persistent decreased endocrine activity, thereby reducing sensitivity to st= ress. It has been recently demonstrated that meditation reduces sympathetic adrenergic r= eceptor sensitivity, producing a decreased response to stressful situations (Mills = et al 1990). Certain studies have also found unique patterns of blood hormone levels = and blood flow to a number of organs including the brain (Jevning & O'Halloran 1984).= Increased levels of gamma aminobutyric acid (GABA), melatonin, and dehydroepiandroste= rone sulfate (DHEA-S) have been reported (Glaser et al 1992, Elias & Wilson 1995= , Massion et al 1995). Meditation is associated with changes in the secretion and rel= ease of several pituitary hormones. The hormonal changes induced by meditation mimi= c the effects of the inhibitory neurotransmitter GABA. Elias and Wilson (1995) hy= pothesize that meditation produces its anxiolytic effects by promoting GABA action in= specific areas of the brain, via a mechanism similar to the effects of synthetic anx= iolytic and tranquilizing agents. Melatonin has been associated with a variety of biolo= gic functions important in maintaining health and preventing disease, and the s= erum level of the adrenal androgen DHEA-S has also been associated with measures= of health and stress. For example, increased levels of DHEA-S has been connect= ed with a reduction in age-related disorders such as cardiovascular diseases and br= east cancer. DHEA-S excretion also decreases in times of stress; since meditator= s have been shown to have an attenuated autonomic response to stressors (Orme-John= son 1973), the higher DHEA-S levels found in during meditation may provide pro= tection against stressor stimulation of the adrenal gland. That the physical effects of meditation persist after the meditation per= iod itself has ended is demonstrated by the fact that hypertension can be effectively cont= rolled by meditation alone without the use of anti-hypertensive drugs (Schneider et a= l 1995). Meditation has also been shown to have long-term effects on the endocrine s= ystem (Werner et al 1986). Another recent study (Zamarra et al 1996) reveals that= meditators have a general increased exercise tolerance and maximal cardiac workload as= compared to non-meditators. Meditation and the Central Nervous System Interestingly, the practice of meditation decreases muscle reflex time (= Warshal 1980, Robertson 1983). Significant reductions in reflex time provides possi= ble neurological evidence for the improved motor performance skills reported in= other studies on meditation, such as higher performance on perceptual-motor speed= tests, static motor performance tests, and physical task tests of balance.[FN3] Me= ditation may somehow accelerate neural conduction or augment the release of neurotransmitters, thereby decreasing synaptic time, resulting in a change = in muscle firing threshold and pattern. These findings appear consistent with the dev= elopment of a heightened sensitivity of the human central nervous system and suggest= a neural mechanism underlying the motor performance improvements of those who medita= te. Studies of brain physiology during meditation have most frequently emplo= yed the electroencephalograph (EEG) for the measurement of brain wave electrical ac= tivity. With most meditative practices the EEG patterns exhibit a slowing and synchronization of brain waves, with alpha waves predominating. More advanc= ed practitioners of meditation demonstrate an even greater slowing of their br= ain waves, with the possible emergence of theta wave patterns.[FN4] These patterns are= consistent with deep relaxation. Alpha rhythm is the classical EEG correlat= e for a state of relaxed wakefulness, also described as relaxed vigilance (Niedermeyer & = Da Silva 1993). Indeed, emotional tension attenuates or blocks the alpha rhythm. The= ta activity is associated with emotional processes and indicates relative matu= rity of the mechanisms linking the cortex, the thalamus, and the hypothalamus; theta rh= ythm also occurs during a state of maximal awareness (Niedermeyer & Da Silva 199= 3). Apparently, an alpha wave pattern is most conducive to creativity and to th= e assimilation of new concepts, while the theta responseseems to be a stage a= t which the mind is capable of deep insights and intuition. It is significant to no= te that practiced meditators can continue to exhibit alpha and theta waves after th= e meditation period has ended (Wallace et al 1971). One study compared different types of breathing during meditation and di= scovered that diaphragmatic, or deep breathing was associated more with an EEG alpha= response than thoracic breathing (Timmons et al 1972). Meditative tradition= s place a great deal of importance on breathing; indeed, breath becomes the object of= awareness in most methods. Specifically, Taoist and Zen traditions of medit= ation have historically placed great value in abdominal breathing, consistent with the= popular belief that the vital center, or hara, is located in the abdomen (Huard 197= 1). The study by Timmons and collaborators validates the merit of deep abdominal br= eathing. The cortex of the brain is popularly believed to consist of two halves, = the left and right hemispheres. Although simplistic, activities such as speech, logical = thinking, analysis, sense of time are thought to function in the left hemisphere, wh= ile the ability to recognize faces and comprehend maps is thought to function in th= e right hemisphere. On the physiological level, it has been demonstrated that the t= wo hemispheres of the cortex are specialized for different modes of informatio= n- processing; the left hemisphere operates primarily in a verbal, intellectua= l, sequential mode, while the right hemisphere operates primarily in a spatial oriented m= ode. The right hemisphere concerns space more than time, and intuition more than log= ic or language. The right lobe also houses the purported center of motor skills c= onnected with spatial awareness. Most people, under scientific measurement, demonstr= ate a marked preponderance towards left hemisphere usage. Several authors hypothesize that systems of meditation alter consciousne= ss by inhibiting cognitive functions associated with the dominant or left cortica= l hemisphere. Ornstein (1975), for example, states that meditation "turns off= " the verbal, linear, analytic style of information processing associated with th= e normal waking state. By inhibiting the left cortical hemisphere, the sense of time= and logic no longer dominate consciousness during meditation. In association with this r= epression of the left hemisphere occurs a hypothesized shift to the right hemispheric= manner of experience, described as holistic, receptive, and beyond language or logic.= Since it is nonlinear, the right cortical hemisphere devalues the concept of cause and = effect. Davidson (1976) argues that meditation leads to the development of right he= misphere associated abilities. This assertion has been verified by several research = projects; meditators show faster reaction times on simple visual reaction time tasks,= thus demonstrating that meditation facilitates right hemisphere specific abiliti= es (Appelle & Oswald 1974, Holt et al 1978, Pagano & Frumkin 1977). Furthermore, EEG alph= a and theta wave coherence is most marked in the right cortical hemisphere during= the practice of meditation (Gaylord et al 1989). Other analyses suggest the existence of synchronization patterns both be= tween corresponding areas of the two cortical hemispheres and within individual hemispheres (Glueck & Stroebel 1978). Some tests indicate that the EEG acti= vation patterns in meditators display a greater flexibility in shifting between he= mispheres in response to the demands of specific tasks (Bennet & Trinder 1977); this rep= resents an integration of the left and right hemispheres of the brain, synchronizing t= he logical with the intuitive. Meditation and Psychology The research literature on meditation suggests that practitioners experi= ence subjective phenomena, such as pronounced feelings of "self-transcendence," = "felt meaning in the world,a heightened sense of connectedness with the world,= " and "a sense of purpose and meaningfulness"[FN5]; these subjective experiences inv= olve radically revised perceptions of self and the external world. Mood changes = include happiness, freedom from anxiety, content with self, and greater vitality. O= ther articles also suggest that meditators gain enhanced confidence, a sense of self-cont= rol, empathy, and self-actualization (Hjelle 1974). Several investigators conclu= de that the practice of meditation improves cognitive task performance, increases menta= l concentration, and reduces susceptibility to stress.[FN6] As described abov= e, many researchers report that meditation reduces the biological components of anx= iety. In general, meditation promotes psychological health (Gaylord et al 1989, Geld= erloos et al 1990). Other psychological consequences of meditation include decreased anger a= roused in high-anger situations (Dua & Swinden 1992) and an increased concentratio= n for mental as well as physical tasks (Dhume & Dhume 1991). Indeed, Davidson et = al (1976) found that experienced meditators had significantly increased attent= ional absorption and that attentional absorption increased as the length of medit= ation experience increased. Long-term meditators appear to possess a more develop= ed ability to voluntarily control attention. A general profile of psychological well-being and perceptual sensitivity= emerges from various studies on meditation. Some of the more commonly reported experiences include amplified perceptual clarity, widened range of psycholo= gical insights, and greater openness to experience.[FN7] As Walsh writes (1984), = "Sensitivity and clarity frequently seem enhanced following a meditation si= tting or retreat. Thus, for example, at these times it seems that I can discriminate= visual forms and outlines more clearly. It also feels as though empathy is signifi= cantly increased and that I am more aware of other people's subtle behaviors, voca= l intonations, etc., as well as my own affective responses to them." One of t= he fundamental objective observations of the enhanced perceptual sensitivity o= f meditators is a decrease in both absolute and discrimination sensory thresh= olds[FN8]; these include a more subtle awareness of previously known concepts and an increased perception of previously unrecognized phenomena. Thus, both subje= ctive and objective examinations agree that meditation enhances perceptual sensit= ivity. Conclusion The concept of meditation arose within the philosophical framework of Ea= stern religious and spiritual disciplines. These traditions practice meditative t= echniques in order to maintain physical health, induce altered states of consciousness, = develop insight, achieve peace, and gain spiritual strength as well as spiritual pu= rification. In these ways, meditation modifies the perception of the world and promotes a = more unified conception of self, nature, and humanity. Martial arts training, by= including the practice of meditation, encourages the development of these attributes = and fosters a more intuitive way of relating to life. Formal meditation refers = to the practice of meditation at specific times, in a specific place and posture, as practi= ced in a Taekwondo dojang. Informal meditation, however, requires no specifications,= but can be practiced at any time and place. The primary goal of meditation in the m= artial arts is not simply to be able to make a meditative effort during formal sittings= , but to maintain and generalize conscious attention to all aspects of martial arts = practice and life in general, thereby eliminating mental tension. Ultimately, the greatest achievement in the martial arts is the simultan= eous refinement of mind and body. The special training of consciousness effectiv= ely regulates every biological system of the body as well as its technical and = mechanical facilities. Cultivation of the mind leads to cultivation of the body, leadi= ng to further cultivation of the mind and so on, eventually attaining an exquisite level = of cooperation and coordination between the two. Notes FN1 Reduced heart rate -- Wallace 1970, Wallace et al 1971, Delmonte 1984, = Zeier 1984, Sudsuang et al 1991, Telles et al 1995 Decreased Blood Pressure -- W= allace et al 1971, Wallace et al 1983, Delmonte 1984, Sudsuang et al 1991, Schneider = et al 1995 Decreased oxygen consumption -- Wallace 1970, Allison 1970, Wallace e= t al 1971, Hirai 1974, Fenwick et al 1977, Zeier 1984, Wilson et al 1987, Benson= et al 1990 Decreased carbon dioxide generation by muscle -- Wallace 1970, Wallac= e et al 1971, Wilson et al 1987, Jevning et al 1992 FN2 Wallace 1970, Wallace et al 1971, Orme-Johnson 1973, Delmonte 1984, Tel= les et al 1995 FN3 Kolb 1974, Orme-Johnson et al 1976, Jedrczak et al 1986, Dhume & Dhume = 1991, Telles et al 1994 FN4 Wallace 1970, Wallace et al 1971, Banquet 1973, Hirai 1974, Corby et al= 1978, Dillbeck & Vesely 1986, Gaylord et al 1989, Jevning et al 1992 FN5 Osis et al 1973, Kohr 1977, Severtsen & Bruya 1986, Bogart 1991 FN6 Blasdell 1973, Orme-Johnson 1973, Appelle & Oswald 1974, Keller & Serag= anian 1984, Severtsen & Bruya 1986, Gaylord et al 1989, Dhume & Dhume 1991, Jin 1= 992, Tsai & Crockett 1993, Janowiak & Hackman 1994, Elias & Wilson 1995, Telles = et al 1995 FN7 Banquet 1973, Osis et al 1973, Shapiro 1980, Walsh 1984, Brown et al 19= 84 FN8 Davidson et al 1976, Brown et al 1984, Freed 1989, Colby 1991 Bibliography Allison J. Respiratory change during transcendental meditation. Lancet = 1:833-4, Apr 1970 Appelle S & Oswald LE. Simple reaction time as a function of alertness a= nd prior mental activity. Perceptual & Motor Skills 38:1263-8, 1974 Banquet JP. Spectral analysis of the EEG in meditation. Electroencephal= ography & Clinical Neurophysiology 35: 143-51, 1973 Bennet JE & Trinder J. Hemispheric laterality and cognitive style associ= ated with transcendental meditation. Psychophysiology 14:293-6, 1977 Benson H. The Relaxation Response. New York: William Morrow & Co, 1975 Benson H, Malhotra MS, Goldman RF, Jacobs GD & Hopkins PJ. Three case re= ports of the metabolic and electroencephalographic changes during advanced Buddhist = meditation techniques. Behav Med 16(2):90-5, Summer 1990 Blasdell KS. The effects of transcendental meditation technique upon a c= omplex perceptual-motor task. In K. Kannelakos and J. Lucas (Eds.), The Psychobiol= ogy of Transcendental Meditation: A Literature Review. Menlo Park: Stanford Resear= ch Institute, 1973. Bogart G. The Use of Meditation in Psychotherapy: A Review of the Litera= ture. Amer J Psychotherapy 45(3):383-412, Jul 1991 Brown D, Forte M & Dysart M. Visual sensitivity and mindfulness meditati= on. Perceptual & Motor Skills 58(3):775-84, Jun 1984 Cauthen N & Prymak C. Meditation versus Relaxation. J Consulting and Cli= nical Psychology 45(3):496-7, 1977 Colby F. An analogue study of the initial carryover effects of meditatio= n, hypnosis, and relaxation using native college students. Biofeedback Self Regulation 16(2):157 65, Jun 1991 Corby JC, Roth WT & Zarzone VP. Psychophysiological correlates of the pr= actice of tantric yoga meditation. Arch Gen Psychiatry 35:571-80, 1978 Davidson JM. The physiology of meditation and mystical states of conscio= usness. Perspectives Biol Med 19:345-79, 1976 Delmont MM. Physiological responses during meditation and rest. Biofeedb= ack Self Regulation 9(2):181-200, Jun 1984 Dhume RR & Dhume RA. A comparative study of the driving effects of dextroamphetamine and yogic meditation on muscle control for the performanc= e of balance on balance board. Indian J Physiol Pharmacology 35(3):191-4, Jul 19= 91 Dillbeck MC & Vesely SA. Participation in the transcendental meditation = program and frontal EEG coherence during concept learning. International J Neurosci= ence 29(1-2):45-55, Mar 1986 Dua JK & Swinden ML. Effectiveness of negative-thought reduction, medita= tion and placebo training treatment in reducing anger. Scandinavian J Psychology 33(= 2):135- 46, 1992 Elias AN & Wilson AF. Serum hormonal concentrations following transcende= ntal meditation: potential role of gamma aminobutyric acid. Med Hypotheses 44(4)= :287- 91, Apr 1995 Fenwick PB, Donaldson S, Gillis L, Bushman J, Fenton GW, Perry I, Tilsle= y C & Serafinowicz H. Metabolic and EEG Changes During Transcendental Meditation:= An Explanation. Biological Psychology 5(2):101-18, 1977 Freed S. Induced Specific Immunological Unresponsiveness & Conditioned behavioral Reflexes, In Functional Isomorphism-Meditation and Conditioned S= pecific Unresponsiveness. International J Neuroscience 44(3-4)275-81, Feb 1989 Gaylord C, Orme-Johnson D & Travis F. The Effects of the Transcendental = Meditation Technique and Progressive Muscle Relaxation on EEG Coherence, St= ress Reactivity, and Mental Health in Black Adults. International J Neuroscience= 46(1):77- 86, May 1989 Gelderloos P, Hermans HG, Ahlscrom HH & Jacoby R. Transcendence and psychological Health: Studies with Long-Term Participants of the Transcendental Meditation = and TM- Sidhi Program. J Psychology 124(2):177-97, Mar 1990 Glaser JL, Brind JL, Vogelman JH, Eisner MJ, Dillbeck MC, Wallace RK Cho= pra D & Orentreich N. Elevated Serum Dehydroepiandrosterone Sulfate Levels in Practitioners= of the Transcendental Meditation and TM-Sidhi programs. J Behavior Med 15(4):327-4= 1, Aug 1992 Glueck BC & Stroebel CF. Meditation in the Treatments of Psychiatric Ill= ness. In A. Sugarman & R. Tarter (Eds.), Expanding Dimensions of Consciousness. New Yor= k: Springer, 1978 Herzog H, Lele VR, Kuwert T, Langen KJ, Kops ER & Feinendegen LE. Change= d pattern of Regional Glucose Metabolism During Yoga Meditative Relaxation. Neuropsychobiology 23(4):182-7, 1990-91 Hirai T. Psychophysiology of Zen. Tokyo: Igaku Shin, 1974 Hjelle LA. Transcendental Meditation and Psychological Health. Perceptua= l & Motor Skills 39:623-8, 1974 Holt WR, Caruso JL & Riley JB. Transcendental Meditation Versus pseudo-M= editation on Visual Choice Reaction Time. Perceptual & Motor Skills 46:726, 1978 Huard P. Oriental Methods of Mental and Physical Fitness: The Complete B= ook of Meditation, Kinesitherapy and Martial Arts in China, India and Japan. New Y= ork: Funk & Wagnalls, 1971 Janowiak JJ & Hackman R. Meditation and College Students' Self-Actualiza= tion and Rated Stress. Psychol Rep 75(2):1007-10, Oct 1994 Jedrczak A, Toomey M & Clements G. The TM-Sidhi Programme, Age, and Brie= f Tests of Perceptual-Motor Speed and Nonverbal Intelligence. J Clinical Psyc= hology 42(1):161-4, Jan 1986 Jevning R, Anand R, Beidebach M & Fernando G. Effects of Regional Cerebr= al Blood Flow on Transcendental Meditation. Physiol Behavior 59(3):399-402, Mar 1996= Jevning R & O'Halloran JP. Metabolic Effects of Transcendental Meditatio= n: Toward a New Paradigm of Neurobiology. In D. Shapiro & R. Walsh (Eds.), Meditation: = Classical and Contemporary Perspectives. New York: Aldine Publishing Co., 1984 Jevning R, Wallace RK & Beidebach M. The Physiology of Meditation: A Wak= eful Hypometabolic Integrated Response. Neuroscience Biobehavior Review 16(3):41= 5-24, Fall 1992 Jin P. Efficacy of Tai Chi, Brisk Walking, Meditation, and Reading in Re= ducing Mental and Emotional Stress. J Psychosomatic Research 36(4):361-70, May 1992 Keller S & Seraganian P. Physical Fitness Level and Autonomic Reactivity= to Psychosocial Stress. J Psychosomatic Research 28(4):279-87, 1984 Kohr E. Dimensionality in the Meditative Experience: A Replication. J Tr= anspersonal Psychology 9(2):193-203, 1977 Kolb DA. Improved Reaction Time Following Transcendental Meditation. In = D.P. Kanellakos & J.S. Lukas (Eds.), The Psychobiology of Transcendental Meditat= ion: A Literature Review. Menlo Park: W.A. Benjamin, 1974 Massion AO, Teas J, Hebert JR, Wertheimer MD & Kabat-Zinn J. Meditation,= Melatonin and Breast/Prostate Cancer: Hypothesis and Preliminary Data. Med = Hypotheses 44(1):39-46, Jan 1995 Mills PJ, Schneider RH, Hill D, Walton KG & Wallace RK. Beta-Adrenergic = Receptor Sensitivity in Subjects Practicing Transcendental Meditation. J Psychosomat= ic Research 34(1):29-33, 1990 Niedermeyer E & Da Silva FL. Electroencephalograpy: Basic Principles, Cl= inical Applications & Related Fields, 3rd ed. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins, 1993 = Orme-Johnson DW. Autonomic Stability and Transcendental Meditation. Psychosomatic Med 35:341-9, 1973 Orme-Johnson DW, Kolb D & Herbert JR. An Experimental Analysis of the Transcendental Meditation Technique on Reaction Time. In D.W. Orme-Johnson = & J.T. Farrow (Eds.), Scientific Research on the Transcendental Meditation Program= : Collected Papers, Vol 1. Livingston Manor: Maharishi International Press, = 1977 Ornstein R. The Psychology of Consciousness. New York: Pelican Books, 19= 75 Osis K, Bokert E & Carlson ML. Dimensions of the Meditative Experience. = J Transpersonal Psychology 5(1):109-35, 1973 Pagano R & Frumkin L. The Effect of Transcendental Meditation on Right hemispheric Functioning. Biofeedback & Self-Regulation. Chicago: Aldine, 1977 Robertson DW. The Short and Long Range Effects of the Transcendental Med= itation technique on Fractionated Reaction Time. J Sports Med Physical Fitness 23(1= ):113-20, Mar 1983 Schneider RH, Staggers F, Alexander CN, Sheppard W, Rainforth M, Kondwan= i K, Smith S & King CG. A Randomised Controlled Trial of Stress Reduction for Hypertension in Older African Americans. Hypertension 26(5):820-7, Nov 1995= Schwartz G. Biofeedback, Self-Regulation, and the Patterning of Physiolo= gical Processes. American Scientist 63:314-25, 1975 Severtsen B & Bruya MA. Effects of Meditation and Aerobic Exercise on EE= G Patterns. J Neuroscience Nursing 18(4):206-10, Aug 1986 Shapiro DH. Meditation: Self-Regulation Strategy & Altered State of Cons= ciousness. New York: Aldine Publishing Co., 1980 Sudsuang R, Chentanez V & Veluvan K. Effect of Buddhist Meditation on Se= rum Cortisol and Total Protein Levels, Blood Pressure, Pulse Rate, Lung Volume = & Reaction Time. Physiology Behavior 50(3):543-8, Sep 1991 Telles S, Hanumanthaiah BH, Nagarathna R & Nagendra HR. Plasticity of Mo= tor Control Systems Demonstrated by Yoga Training. Indian J Physiol Pharmacolog= y 38(2):143-4, Apr 1994 Telles S, Nagarathna R & Nagendra HR. Autonomic Changes During "OM" Meditation. Indian J Physiol Pharmacology 39(4):418-20, Oct 1995 Timmons B, Salamy J, Kamiya J & Girton D. Abdominal, Thoracic Respirator= y Movements and Levels of Arousal. Psychonomic Science 27:173-5, 1972. Tsai SL & Crockett MS. Effects of Relaxation Training, Combining Imagery= , and Meditation on the Stress Level of Chinese Nurses Working in Modern Hospi= tals in Taiwan. Issues Mental Health Nursing 14(1):51-66, Jan-Mar 1993 Wallace RK. Physiological Effects of Transcendental Meditation. Science = 167:1751- 4, 1970 Wallace RK, Benson H & Wilson A. A Wakeful Hypometabolic Physiologic Sta= te. Am J Physiol 221:795-9, 1971 Wallace RK, Silver J, Mills PJ, Dillbeck MC & Wagoner DE. Systolic Blood= Pressure and Long Term Practice of the Transcendental Meditation and TM-Sidhi Program: E= ffects of TM on Systolic Blood Pressure. Psychosomatic Med 45(1):41-6, Mar 1983 Walsh R. Initial Meditative Experiences. In D. Shapiro & R. Walsh, (Eds.= ), Meditation: Classical and Contemporary Perspectives. New York: Aldine Publishing Co., 1= 984 Warshal D. Effects of the Transcendental Meditation Technique on Normal = and Jendrassik Reflex Time. Perceptual & Motor Skills 50:1103-6, 1980 Werner OR, Wallace RK, Charles B, Janssen G, Stryker T & Chalmers RA. Lo= ng-Term Endocrinologic Changesin Subjects Practicing the Transcendental Meditation = and TM- Sidhi program. Psychosomatic Med 48(1-2):59-66, Jan-Feb 1986 Wilson AF, Jevning R & Guich S. Marked Reduction of Forearm Carbon Dioxi= de Production During States of Decreased Metabolism. Physiology Behavior 41(4)= :347- 52, 1987 Zamarra JW, Schneider RH, Besseghini I, Robinson DK & Salerno JW. Useful= ness of the Transcendental Meditation Program in the Treatment of Patients with Cor= onary Artery Disease. Amer J Cardiology 77(10):867-70, Apr 1996 Zeier H. Arousal Reduction with Biofeedback-Supported Respiratory Medita= tion. Biofeedback Self Regulation 9(4):497-508, Dec 1984 Quote Link to comment Share on other sites More sharing options...
Recommended Posts
Join the conversation
You are posting as a guest. If you have an account, sign in now to post with your account.
Note: Your post will require moderator approval before it will be visible.