Guest guest Posted December 16, 2004 Report Share Posted December 16, 2004 HinduThought, Srinivasan Kalyanaraman <kalyan97@g...> wrote: Let me explain why I hold IE linguistics to be a fraudulent discipline, not unlike Freudian psychoanalysis which stands discredited. IE linguistics has become a messed up eurocentric exercise ignoring the I in the IE as a result of which there is little understanding of the evolution of languages of Bharat. Remember Burrow's conundrum after compiling Dravidian Etymological Dictionary? He couldn't figure out the nature of the borrowings between the so-called Dravidian and so-called IA etyma and ended up speculating that there could have been a linguistic area in Bharat where Munda, IA and Dravidian dialects interacted in a dialectical continuum. So much for the achievement of IE linguistics in relation to languages in Bharat. More devastating is the absence of adequate study of semantics in the discipline. Arbitrary, anecdotal exercises result in a falsifiable * hypothetical constructions of a hypothetical PIE language. The impact of the ice age on European settlements has not even been taken into account. In contrast to the straight-jacket evolution of IE linguistics, Bharatiya language studies have delved into the issue of general semantics. One issue in semantics is sphota. The term sphoTa is etymologically derived from the rootsphuT, which means 'to burst', or become suddenly rent asunder (with a sound) . The word sphoTa is explained in two ways [ii]. Naagesha BhaTTa defines sphoTa as sphuTati prakaashate'rtho'smaad iti sphoTaH (that, from which the meaning bursts forth, that is, shines forth. In other words, the word that expresses a meaning, or the process of expressing a meaning through a word is called sphoTa. SphoTa, according to Maadhava, is that which is manifested or revealed by the phonemes: sphuTyate vyajyate varNairiti sphoTaH. [unquote] http://www.languageinindia.com/june2004/anirbansphota1.html Sphota is a permanent element of s'abda. The very compound padaartha = pada + artha shows the clear enunciation of a word as a semantic indicator. This is echoed by Tolkaappiyan: ella_ccollum porul. kur-ittanave 'all words are semantic indicators'. If IE linguistics has pursued this line of understanding of evolution of words, sentences (cf. Bhartrhari's Vaakyapadiya), there could have been some progress in language studies. IE linguistics is a classic case of wasted opportunity wallowing in phonemic juggleries. This is one example of a pseudo-science failing to follow a clear path to truth. Kalyanaraman Eureka! PD Smith enjoys John Waller's iconoclastic history of scientific endeavour, Leaps in the Dark PD Smith Saturday December 11, 2004 The Guardian Leaps in the Dark: The Making of Scientific Reputations by John Waller 291pp, Oxford, £18.99 "Unhappy is the land that needs heroes," says Galileo in Bertolt Brecht's great play about the Italian physicist, and John Waller couldn't agree more. In Fabulous Science (2002) Waller showed how science was a series of "powerful human dramas in which naked ambition has at least as big a role as technical virtuosity". In his latest book he adopts an equally iconoclastic approach. Once again he takes aim at the heroes of science, firing a broadside at recent popular histories that follow a familiar formula: "The hero arrives at a new idea (Act I), suffers the wrath of jealousy, conservatism, and clerical bigotry (Act II), and is then triumphantly vindicated (Act III)." According to Waller, scientific discovery is a "multi-participant event", not a story of lone heroes. Another trusty cliché of popular science writing is the "eureka" moment. The falling apple that supposedly inspired the theory of gravitation was a myth. As Waller points out, the devious Newton probably used it as a ploy to avoid acknowledging any of his contemporaries. Such moments make a great story, but are bad history. Waller has an academic historian's disdain for the simplifying instincts of popular writers: "Cutting-edge science is usually a messy, difficult and uncertain business." It's only with hindsight that everything seems clear-cut: "Those in the right are then hailed as heroes and their erstwhile rivals damned as bloody-minded egoists." To set the record straight, Waller knocks a few scientific greats off their pedestals and revives the fortunes of some "also-rans". Scientists now scorned for backing the wrong horse often followed impeccable methods. Joseph Glanvill was a respected member of the Royal Society in the 17th century, and a leading figure in the scientific revolution. But he believed in witches and demons. To him the facts were clear: the "Attestation of Thousands of Eye and Ear witnesses" proved the devil was up to his old tricks. It's easy now to dismiss such beliefs as irrelevant superstition, but Waller argues that they are integral to the age and thus the science (or natural philosophy as it was then). Glanvill's approach was rigorously sceptical and scientific, but in the 1600s the world was a strange place, full of unexplained occult forces. Even Newton couldn't explain the mysterious power of gravity, yet no one doubted its existence. Glanvill's beliefs were "no more fanciful than the conclusions reached by countless other bona fide scientists, before and since". Leaps in the Dark succeeds wonderfully in revealing the subtlety of the scientific process. Science does not follow a royal road to truth; a meandering river is a more accurate analogy. The history of science often tells us more about ourselves and our relation to the world, than it does about the discovery of truth - that is why it is so endlessly fascinating. There may be few heroes in Waller's account, but there are certainly villains, those more interested in their reputations than the scientific method. As he points out, "science, like politics, has its share of propagandists". The moment when the father of psychiatry, Philippe Pinel, unchained the inmates of the madhouse of Bicêtre in 1793 has been immortalised in paint and stone. It captures perfectly the Rousseauesque ideals of the French revolution and the enlightened approach of science. There's just one problem - it never happened, says Waller. Other myths serve more selfish purposes. Sir Robert Watson-Watt created an image of himself as a scientific hero, the self-proclaimed "Father of Radar", whose invention saved Britain from Hitler's Luftwaffe. Watson-Watt's team developed a basic but effective radar system that was indeed crucial during the Battle of Britain. But after the war, this "self-centred" man systematically expunged the contributions of others. He portrayed himself as the sole discoverer of "one of the most important technological innovations of the 20th century", even though it had been around as early as 1904. As Waller rightly says, "no achievement in science is exclusively the product of one brain". They are words that should be framed and hung above every science writer's desk. PD Smith's illustrated biography of Einstein is published by Haus. http://books.guardian.co.uk/reviews/scienceandnature/0,6121,1371059,0 0.html --- End forwarded message --- Quote Link to comment Share on other sites More sharing options...
Recommended Posts
Join the conversation
You are posting as a guest. If you have an account, sign in now to post with your account.
Note: Your post will require moderator approval before it will be visible.